Doayâu
To Be Continued... Cedh is still working on this article. The contents are incomplete and likely to undergo changes. |
Doayâu [ dɔ̀ɐ̯.jɑ́ʊ̯ ] | |
Period | c. -500 YP |
Spoken in | Naunupai peninsula |
Total speakers | unknown (~12,000 in Pítau) |
Writing system | adapted Lukpanic script |
Classification | Western languages Coastal Western Doayâu |
Typology | |
Basic word order | mixed |
Morphology | agglutinating w/ some fusion |
Alignment | split-S |
Credits | |
Created by | Cedh |
Doayâu [ dɔ̀ɐ̯.jɑ́ʊ̯ ] is a Western language spoken on much of the Naunupai peninsula (< Fu Pitão nãu lupãi 'land facing the sea'), especially around the cities of Pítau and Sìmésita, around -500 YP. The language is a direct descendant of Proto-Coastal-Western. Its closest relatives are Ishoʻu ʻOhu, the language of Isi (to the west of Pítau), and Ìletlégbàku, spoken around Namù (to the north). The relationship with Ìletlégbàku is particularly close; the two languages share some early sound changes. However, differences in phonology and grammar are considerable despite geographical proximity, to the point that the Doayâu and Ìletlégbàku are not mutually intelligible.
The name Doayâu derives from the PCW appellation duğàyòbu 'good mouth' (named in contrast to the 'bad mouth' of other Coastal Western dialects).
Phonology
Consonants
labial | alveolar | palatal | velar | glottal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
plosive | p · b | t · d | k · ɡ | ||
affricate | ts | ||||
fricative | ɸ | s | h | ||
nasal | m | n | ŋ | ||
liquid | l · ɾ | j |
- /ɡ ɸ ŋ ɾ j/ are written g f ng r y.
Vowels
front | central | back | |
---|---|---|---|
close | i | u | |
mid | e | o | |
open | a |
- Vowels may combine into diphthongs freely; however, no combinations of close and mid vowels occur. ea ai au oa are by far the most common diphthongs.
Syllable structure
Doayâu has a very simple (C)V(V/N) syllable structure. An empty onset may only occur word-initially; a coda nasal may only appear medially after a single vowel. The only permitted consonant clusters are mb nd (former *ŋg has been simplified to [ŋ], which is still written ng). Some scholars have proposed to analyse these as phonemic prenasalised plosives; however, all occurrences in native words are a transparent result of vowel deletion, and so they will be treated as clusters for the purposes of this document.
Tone
There are three phonemic tones. High tone is marked with an acute accent (í é á ó ú), low tone is marked with a grave accent (ì è à ò ù), and mid tone is unmarked.
- On diphthongs, the high tone is realised as rising, unless the preceding syllable has high tone or a phonetic contour tone.
- The mid and low tones have merged on diphthongs, resulting in a falling pitch contour unless the previous syllable has low tone or a pitch contour, in which case the diphthong itself exhibits low pitch. Low/falling tone on diphthongs is not marked in writing since it is the default; high/rising tone on diphthongs is marked with a circumflex accent (e.g. êa).
- The mid and low tones also merge on monophthongs if the previous syllable has high or rising tone. In such positions, an orthographic grave accent indicates that the tone always remains low even if followed by a tone-raising suffix, which would cause a vowel without a diacritic to become high-pitched.
Phonetic detail
- t d ts s n l are palatalised to [tʲ dʲ tsʲ sʲ nʲ lʲ] or even [c ɟ tɕ ɕ ɲ ʎ] before i, and sometimes also before e. For the most part this is fully predictable, but some speakers maintain a phonemic difference: palatalised coronals before [i] from earlier *i and before [ea] from *ie *ia, but plain coronals before [i] from earlier *ə and before [ea] from *ɛː *ɛa *ɛɔ.
- b d g may become [β ð ɣ] when preceded by a low-tone syllable. If the next vowel is i, [ð] may further palatalise to [ʝ].
- g is also often slightly labialised, which results in some speakers merging e eo and a au when followed by g.
- In the vicinity of a, the glottal fricative h may be pronounced as any of [h x χ ħ], the last of these being the most common realisation.
- f may be pronounced [f], especially near high-tone vowels.
- High tone is correlated with a slight aspiration on preceding voiceless obstruents.
- Falling tone diphthongs are creaky-voiced before voiced consonants other than y.
- In careful speech, diphthongs are often pronounced as disyllabic vowel sequences. This is most common for ea eo oe oa.
- oa merges into oe before y and when the next syllable contains i. Similarly, ea merges into eo before labial consonants and when the next syllable contains u.
- i always dissimilates to e after y.
Handling loanwords
Doayâu has borrowed many words from various Lukpanic languages, most of them from the Kpitamoa dialect. Other sources for loanwords are Ìletlégbàku and Ishoʻu ʻOhu, both of them closely related to Doayâu itself, and the more remotely related Western languages Šetâmol and Hośər.
The following table shows how non-native phones are normally adopted into Doayâu. In older loans, different correspondences may occur.
|
|
The stress accent of Lukpanic or Hośər words is usually reflected as a high tone, unless originally followed by a sonorant.
Sound changes from Proto-Coastal-Western
Morphophonological processes
Vowel alternations
Doayâu exhibits prominent remnants of two metaphonic processes (traditionally called "i-affection" and "u-affection" in Coastal Western linguistic studies), which affected vowels when the next syllable contained *j *i or *w *u respectively. Originally, i-affection simply fronted and raised non-high vowels, and u-affection backed and rounded front vowels. However, the situation in Doayâu is much more complex than that. The original shifts took place long before the PCW stage, and their original phonetic conditioning environment has often become obscured by further changes. What is more, Doayâu has since experienced a vowel shift that reduced non-metaphonised *a to *ə, which was then subject to secondary assimilation processes whose outcome is usually i or u, while *e (often from i-affected *a) and *o (often from u-affected *a) both lowered to a. The rather confusing result is that the actual pronunciations of metaphonised vowels may be exactly the opposite of what would be expected from the traditional label.
For the purposes of this grammar, a simpler terminology based on the concept of ablaut grades will be used. No single stem has more than three different vowels across the paradigm (most have only two), but the patterns of overlap clearly point to four distinct grades in Doayâu morphophonology. Three of them etymologically correspond to plain, i-affected, and u-affected vowels, and the fourth, "strong" grade has evolved from a distinct development of vowels in word-final position and before coda *h. All grades are morphologically conditioned.
Based on their ablaut pattern, almost all inflectable morphemes in Doayâu can be sorted in one of several stem classes. The citation form is the basic grade for verbal and adjectival stems (because these never appear in the strong grade), and the strong grade for all other lexeme classes. This is supplemented in the lexicon by the stem type label for all lexemes that may undergo vowel alternation.
simple stems | N-stems | diphthong stems | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
i | e | e/a | a | ə | o/a | o | u | N | ai | ui | ea | oa | |||
strong grade | i, e | e | e | a | a | a, o | o | o, u | a | ai | ui | ea, eo | oa, oe | ||
basic grade | i | e | a | a | i, u¹ | a | o | u | nas² | ai | ui | ea, eo | oa, oe | ||
i-grade | i | i | i | e | a | a | e | u | a | ai | ui | ea | oe | ||
u-grade | u | o | a | o | a | a | o | u | a | au | ù | eo | oa |
- ¹) u appears adjacent to a labial consonant or g from former *w; i appears elsewhere.
- ²) N-stems are basically a subclass of the ə-stems that trigger nasal mutation (see below) in the basic grade when followed by a consonant that can undergo this process. When nasal mutation applies, vowels in the preceding syllable that derive from *ə turn into u before mb if not preceded by a labial consonant or g from former *w, and into i otherwise.
When a high-tone stem vowel of the classes e a o is followed by an affix that triggers tone-lowering, the stem vowel changes to a regardless of the ablaut grade; e.g. tsufé- "to find" + low-gù- comes out as tsufagù-.
Stems ending in iu au, nouns with a strong grade in u, and most borrowed words in a do not exhibit ablaut. The former three may be subsumed under the u-stems (although nouns in iu au behave a bit different from nouns in monophthongal u), and the latter under the o/a-stems.
Stems ending in monophthongal i or u change these into e o when a suffix starting with one of e a o is added, forming a diphthong.
Some words have irregular vocalism in one or more of the grades. Such exceptions are indicated in the lexicon.
Tone sandhi
Some inflectional and derivational affixes modify the tone of the preceding vowel. Both raising and lowering occur; these are indicated with the labels high and low respectively.
Lenition
Lenition (len) affects word-initial consonants. It occurs on nouns when an adjectival prefix, a possessive prefix or a quantifier proclitic is added, on transitive verbs when an inanimate noun is incorporated, and on the second member of compounds with a nominal or verbal head. Recent loanwords may occasionally resist lenition in higher-register speech.
original | lenited |
---|---|
b | g, ع |
d | r, l, y, ز |
g | y, س |
- ¹) Lenited b is usually zero adjacent to an original rounded vowel (except before a diphthong), and g elsewhere.
- ²) The outcome of lenited d is highly unpredictable since it can derive from any of *d *dz *dɮ in native words. For loanwords, y is the most common variant.
- ³) Lenited g is generally zero before i u. Most other instances of g are immune to lenition because historical *ɣ eventually merged back into /ɡ/, but a few words have y instead.
Aspiration
Aspiration (asp) also affects word-initial consonants. It occurs on transitive verbs with the incorporation of animate nouns.
original | aspirated |
---|---|
p, m | f |
t, n | f, s¹ |
ts | s |
k, ng | h |
b | p |
d | t, ts² |
g | k, f³ |
- ¹) f occurs before historical *o *u; s occurs elsewhere.
- ²) ts appears where the verb historically had *dz *dɮ; t appears where it had *d.
- ³) k appears with etymological *g, and f appears with etymological *w. Instances of g that derive from PCW *ɣ do not undergo aspiration.
Labial mutation
Labial mutation (lab) is triggered by syllables which historically contained *o or *u. By this process, certain preceding consonants are assimilated to the labial place of articulation.
original | labialised |
---|---|
k | p |
g | b¹ |
h | f |
s | f² |
ng | m |
- ¹) g undergoes labialisation only on rare occasions, because lenited instances of historical *b became velarised (*β > *w > *ɣ) and eventually merged back into /ɡ/.
- ²) Some instances of s are immune to labialisation.
Prenasalisation
Stems that historically ended in a nasal consonant followed by *ə sometimes trigger prenasalisation (nas) on subsequent consonants. The vowel in between is dropped, and the two consonants coalesce into a homoorganic cluster of a nasal and a voiced plosive; in the case of ng this is further simplified to a velar nasal.
The most common application of prenasalisation is with the addition of certain adjectival prefixes to a noun. It also occurs on some suffixes when they are added to verbal stems of the relevant phonological shape. A number of older nominal compounds exhibit prenasalisation as well; however, the process is no longer productive for new-formed compounds.
original | prenasalised |
---|---|
p, b | mb |
t, ts, d | nd |
r, l, y | nd¹ |
k | ng |
g | ng, mb² |
- ¹) Prenasalisation of r l y occurs only in certain suffixes, not on stem-initial consonants.
- ²) g usually becomes ng, but instances that derive from earlier *w become mb instead.
Morphology
Nouns
Case and number
Doayâu nouns inflect for three cases (absolutive, ergative and oblique) and two numbers (singular vs. plural). There is also an animacy distinction, which manifests itself in a slightly different inflection pattern. Most notably, inanimate nouns do not have a morphological ergative case.
The endings and the ablaut grades they select are as follows:
animate | inanimate | grade | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
singular | absolutive | -Ø | -Ø | strong |
ergative | -ì | i | ||
oblique | -ù | -ù | u | |
plural | absolutive | -´ | -´ | basic |
ergative | -si | i | ||
oblique | -fu | -ú | u |
- The erg.sg and obl.sg endings replace any stem-final vowel that is neither a nor u (nor, in the oblique case, ì).
- The abs.pl ending is simply a floating high tone that is attached to the basic grade of the stem vowel. Nouns whose stem vowel has high tone already insert epenthetic -gV- to carry the extra tone, where V denotes an echo copy of the preceding vowel. Diphthongs copy only their offglide; after monophthongal i e the epenthetic syllable appears as -yé-.
- The oblique case is often built on a different stem due to labialisation effects of historical *u *o.
- Stems in low-tone ì form their oblique singular as a diphthong -iu (and the inanimate plural with -îu).
- All vowel-initial suffixes gain an epenthetic -g- when preceded by a diphthong, which coalesces with the offglide of ai ui into -y-. The erg.sg of stems in ai ui is -aye -uye.
The table below shows the most common nominal inflection patterns, sorted by stem type.
simple stems | diphthong stems | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
i | e | e/a | a | ə | o/a | o | u | ai | ui | ea | oa | iu | au | |||
singular | absolutive | -i, -e | -e | -e | -a | -a, -í | -a, -o | -o | -o, -u | -ai | -ui | -ea, -eo | -oa, -oe | -iu | -au | |
ergative (a.) | -ì | -ì | -ì | -ì | -ai | -ai | -ì | -ui | -aye | -uye | -eagì | -oegì | -iugì | -augì | ||
oblique | -ù | -ù | -au | -ù | -au | -au | -ù | -ù | -ayù | -uyù | -eogù | -oagù | -iugù | -augù | ||
plural | absolutive | -í | -é | -á | -á | -í, -ú¹ | -á | -ó | -ú | -âi | -ûi | -êa, -êo | -ôa, -ôe | -îu | -âu | |
ergative (a.) | -isi | -isi | -asi | -esi | -asi | -asi | -esi | -usi | -aisi | -uisi | -easi | -oesi | -iusi | -ausi | ||
oblique (a.) | -ufu | -ofu | -afu | -ofu | -afu | -afu | -ofu | -ufu | -aufu | -ùfu | -eofu | -oafu | -iufu | -aufu | ||
oblique (i.) | -ú | -ú | -âu | -ú | -âu | -âu | -ú | -ú | -ayú | -uyú | -eogú | -oagú | -iugú | -augú |
- ¹) -ú appears after a labial consonant or g from former *w; -í appears elsewhere.
Some examples of fully inflected nouns follow.
Animate examples:
i | e/a | a | ə | o/a | u | au | oa | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
'horse' | 'woman' | 'person' | 'animal' | 'priest' | 'wolf' | 'sheep' | 'leader' | ||
singular | absolutive | sàre | ìrè | yùmuyá | dàfa | sìlugo | bufo | mâu | fôa |
ergative | sàrì | ìrì | yùmuyè¹ | dàfai | sìlugai | bufui | maugì | foegì | |
oblique | sàrù | ìrau | yùmuyù | dàfau | sìlugau | bufù | maugù | foagù | |
plural | absolutive | sàrí | ìrá | yùmuyágá² | dàfú | sìlugá | bufú | mâugú² | fôagú² |
ergative | sàrisi | ìrìsi | yùmuyési | dàfasi | sìlugasi | bufusi | mâusi | fôesi | |
oblique | sàrufu | ìràfu | yùmuyófu | dàfafu | sìlugafu | bufufu | mâufu | fôafu |
- ¹) The seemingly unexpected -è in this form is the result of regular /ji/-dissimilation.
- ²) Note the epenthetic -gV- to carry the additional high tone.
Inanimate examples:
i | e | e/a | ə | o/a | u | ea | ai | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
'river' | 'water' | 'sky' | 'earth' | 'smoke' | 'forest' | 'garden' | 'night' | ||
singular | absolutive | yàri | difé | dure | tsaka | lugo | kápàpù | tsea | sai |
oblique | yàrù | difù | durau | tsapau | lugau | kápàpù | tseogù | sayù | |
plural | absolutive | yàrí | diféyé¹ | durá | tsakí | lugá | kápàpú | tsêa | sâi |
oblique | yàrú | difú | durâu | tsapâu | lugâu | kápàpú | tsêogú | sayú |
- ¹) Note the epenthetic -yV- to carry the additional high tone.
Clitic postpositions
In contrast to its ancestor PCW, Doayâu mostly uses prepositions. However, some of the original Coastal Western postpositions are still in use; these now behave as enclitics that attach to their object. Clitic postpositions can be thought of as an extended case system; however, they are generally optional, and even with nouns in an indirect object role, which mandatorily take a postposition, there is usually a choice about which clitic to use.
All of these enclitics govern the oblique case for the noun phrase they attach to.
postposition | gloss |
---|---|
-ga, -a¹ | 'for' (dative-benefactive) |
-re | 'to' (dative-allative) |
-tuge | 'at, in' (locative-inessive) |
-ní | 'on, touching' (locative-adessive) |
-yeogu | 'away from' (ablative) |
-má, -amá² | 'with, by' (comitative-instrumental) |
-gefa, -ifa³ | 'without' (privative) |
- ¹) -ga appears after diphthongs; -a appears after monophthongal u, giving the combined form -oa.
- ²) -má appears after diphthongs; -amá appears after monophthongal u, giving the combined form -oamá.
- ³) -gefa appears after diphthongs; -ifa appears otherwise.
The table lists only clitics that can still head a postpositional phrase. Several other morphemes that historically also functioned as postpositions have developed into derivational suffixes instead; these are given in the appropriate section of this grammar sketch.
Possession
Possession is marked with prefixes, which agree with the possessor's number, person and animacy. The PCW distinction between alienable and inalienable types of possession has been lost; possessive prefixes are now used with all nouns. If the possessor is overtly given, it normally precedes the possessed, and is cast in the oblique case.
The inherited alternation between regular, i-affected, and u-affected prefixes has mostly been leveled away by sound change and analogy, with only a few irregularities remaining.
singular | plural | |
---|---|---|
1st | ni- | si- |
2nd | da- | tsa- |
3rd | a- | sa-/ga- |
- Nouns beginning with a voiced plosive usually undergo lenition with the addition of a possessive prefix.
- All possessive prefixes that begin with a consonant lose their vowel before vowel-initial stems, with a stem-initial monophthongal vowel acquiring low tone in compensation. Stems beginning with the sequences /ji je ja jo ju/ behave like stems beginning with i e ea eo iu in this regard.
- Before nasal consonants, all possessive prefixes receive low tone.
- The 1st person prefixes change their vowel to u before a labial consonant.
- The 2nd person prefixes change their vowel to i if the first syllable of the possessed noun also contains i.
- The 3rd person singular prefix becomes i- before nouns beginning with a Ci sequence. If the noun begins with a vowel, the prefix coalesces with stem-initial i u into ai au, and with stem-initial e a o into the diphthong ea. Before diphthongs, the prefix appears as ay-.
- In the 3rd person plural, sa- is used for animate possessors, and ga- is used for inanimate possessors. If the first syllable of the possessed noun contains u, these prefixes undergo labialisation to fa- and ba- respectively.
Pronouns
Personal pronouns
Doayâu has personal pronouns for the first and second persons only. Third person arguments are referenced with demonstratives instead. The personal pronouns inflect for case and number more or less like ordinary animate nouns; however, they feature an additional distributive-partitive plural form.
absolutive | ergative | oblique | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person | singular | na | nai | nau |
plural | nà | nasi | nafu | |
distributive | sa | sai | sau | |
2nd person | singular | ta | tai | tau |
plural | tà | tasi | tafu | |
distributive | tí | tì | tù |
Demonstratives
The two proximate demonstrative pronouns of PCW, *dze- and *da-, have merged in many forms due to sound change, and were eventually combined into a single paradigm. With this, the system of demonstratives has been simplified to three levels of deixis. There are also a special pronoun for the introduction of sentence topics, which derives from the PCW cataphoric stem *gi-, an interrogative pronoun, which derives from the PCW interrogative particle *ği compounded with the anaphoric stem *ja-, and emphatic forms of the regular demonstratives, which derive from compounding with the cataphoric stem. All the emphatic demonstratives can also function as obviative pronouns whenever two different 3rd person arguments need to be distinguished. The topic marker and the interrogative pronoun do not have an emphatic variant.
Deictics most commonly appear on their own in pronominal function, but they can also be used coreferentially within a noun phrase. All demonstratives must take a classifier suffix (see below for details) and a suffix for case and number agreement.
regular | emphatic | |
---|---|---|
topic marker | gi- | – |
proximate | da- | dì- |
medial | tsé- | tséye-¹ |
distal | tá- | tai- |
interrogative | gea- | – |
- ¹) The u-grade form of tséye- is tseo-.
Classifiers
Doayâu retains seven of the ten PCW noun classifiers. Their association with animacy has become completely fixed, so that the system may now be analysed as full noun class agreement.
Several classifiers trigger specific ablaut grades for preceding syllables, and all of them are subject to morphophonemic alternations themselves.
class | affix | stem type | triggers | refers to | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
animate | I | -go- | o/a | lab, u | humans, gods, spirits, birds, tools |
II | -ta- | ə | animals, other animate nouns | ||
inanimate | III | -eatá-¹ | a | i | limited areas, settlements, buildings, boats |
IV | -ri- | i | i | food, ideas, unlimited areas, mass nouns | |
V | -tsú- | u | u | soft non-food items | |
VI | -i-, -ye-² | i | i | liquids, air, fire | |
VII | -tsa- | ə | solid things, other inanimate nouns |
- ¹) Classifier III replaces any preceding monophthongal stem vowel except u, and surfaces as -yeatá- after u or a diphthong. All diphthongs except au are simplified to their first component vowel before this morpheme.
- ²) Classifier VI combines with preceding stem vowels of the types a, ə, and o/a into -ai-, and with u into -ui-. The resulting stem then inflects according to the diphthong formed. Other monophthongal stem vowels merge with the classifier morpheme into -ì-. After diphthongs, the marker surfaces as -ye-.
Numerals
Cardinal numbers take obligatory classifier suffixes to agree with their nominal referent. Ordinals do not take a classifier when used attributively, but do so when used as a pronoun. Attributive numerals always precede their referent. For counting, the bare stem is used.
Like other Western languages, Doayâu uses a base eight number system.
cardinal | ordinal | |
---|---|---|
0 | ye | – |
1 | tugo- | tugau |
2 | ri- | ruyù |
3 | naupu- | naupù |
4 | màtsa- | màtsau |
5 | oa- | oagù |
6 | mìri- | mìrù |
7 | nairi- | nairù |
108 | nigo- | nigau |
Numbers above 108 are built with a special combining form of the base, neo-, which is prefixed to the ordinal or cardinal (as appropriate) of the lower order. Before oa-, an epenthetic -g- is inserted: neogoa- '158 / thirteen'. Multiples of 108 are formed with a preposed ordinal of the higher order: naupù neonairi- '378 / thirty-one'. '1008 / sixty-four' is neonigo-.
Indefinite quantifiers
Indefinite quantifiers can appear in two forms. When used as free-standing pronouns, they take obligatory classifier suffixes and regularly inflect for number and case. When used attributively, they attach to their head noun as proclitics, triggering lenition on stem-initial voiced plosives, and undergoing tone lowering before stem-initial nasals (but not any other morphophonological processes).
The most important indefinite quantifiers are:
- fá- 'all, every'
- lìtsi- 'many'
- ripu- 'an average number of'
- tupi- 'some, a few'
- ye- 'no, none'
- yalu- 'other'
- ka- 'each one' (distributive)
- gi- 'which' (interrogative; gea- as a pronoun)
A partitive meaning can be achieved by using a quantifier pronoun in the oblique case.
- tupiu
- tupi-i-ù
- some-IV-OBL
- tùmí
- tùmì-´
- grape-ABS.PL
Verbs
Verbal morphology in Doayâu is mostly agglutinative, although this may be obscured a bit by morphophonemic alternations of vowels and consonants. The general structure of Doayâu verbs can be summarised like this:
verb stem | evidential | (modals) | participant marking | |||||
(incorporated noun) |
root | (derivational suffixes) |
dir sens evid ass rep |
q neg |
aff dub |
opt | 1p.abs 2p.abs I.abs II.abs III.abs IV.abs V.abs VI.abs VII.abs |
Ø 1p.erg 2p.erg 3p.erg |
Evidentials
Due to phonological erosion and analogical interference from the agglutinative verbal system of the Lukpanic languages, verbs in Doayâu may now be morphologically unmarked for evidentiality. On closer inspection, however, evidential marking is still compulsory: a verb without an overt evidential morpheme is generally interpreted as referring to an action that the speaker was directly involved in, either as a core participant of the verb, or else as a member of a group that acted as a core participant collectively. For a different meaning, an overtly marked evidential suffix must be used. Also, even the direct participation evidential does have a non-zero allomorph -yá-, which appears in negated verbs.
suffix | stem type | triggers | |
---|---|---|---|
direct participation (dir) | -Ø-/-yá-¹ | (a) | |
sensory perception (sens) | -ri- | i | i |
inferred from evidence (evid) | -lu- | u | lab, u |
assumption; guess (ass) | -ya-/-ga-² | e/a | |
hearsay; fiction (rep) | -gù-/-gà-³ | ə | low |
- ¹) -yá- appears only in negated verbs.
- ²) -ga- occurs before suffixes that trigger u-grade, in negated verbs, and after u o; -ya- appears elsewhere. In interrogative verbs, this evidential has a zero allomorph.
- ³) -gà- occurs before suffixes that trigger i- or u-grade; -gù- appears elsewhere.
When added to a verb stem that triggers prenasalisation, the sens marker becomes -ndi-, the ass marker becomes -nga-, and the rep marker becomes -mbù- or -mbà-.
Modality
Four different modal suffixes can optionally appear in the Doayâu verb, following the evidentials and preceding participant agreement. The interrogative and negative morphemes give information about the truth value of the proposition on the speech act level; the affirmative and dubitative affixes indicate certainty or doubt about the proposition on the part of the speaker. Modal suffixes of these two groups may be combined; in this case the speech act marker precedes the mirativity marker.
In addition to this, Doayâu has borrowed the intentional suffix -mau- from the local Lukpanic dialect. This morpheme (glossed as optative) can be used to indicate various meanings, among them plans, wishes, volitional actions, future events, or imperatives. It is only rarely combined with an evidential, but is frequently preceded by one or even two of the other modality suffixes.
suffix | stem type | triggers | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
A | interrogative (q) | -gí-/-yé-¹ | i | |
negative (neg) | -fu- | ə | i, high | |
B | affirmative (aff) | -pù- | u | u, low |
dubitative (dub) | -ù-² | u | ||
C | optative (opt) | -mau- | au | low |
- ¹) -yé- appears after i e; and -gí- appears elsewhere.
- ²) The dubitative marker replaces preceding i u, and coalesces with any other preceding vowel into -au-.
Participant marking
Doayâu verbs agree in person and noun class (the latter only for 3rd person arguments) with their primary absolutive argument. Transitive verbs are additionally marked for the person (but not noun class) of their ergative argument. The number of participants is not marked on the verb.
Participant markers follow the evidentiality suffixes, with absolutive markers preceding ergative markers. The underlying morphemes are as follows:
absolutive | ergative | |
---|---|---|
1st person | -na-¹,² | -ne² |
2nd person | -tá-³ | -tsé³ |
3rd person | (classifier) | -ga/-a⁴ |
- ¹) The 1st person absolutive marker loses its vowel when followed by a 2nd or 3rd person ergative marker.
- ²) Both 1st person markers cause tone lowering on the preceding vowel.
- ³) When preceded by u o, the 2nd person markers appear in the allomorphs -ká- (absolutive) and -ké (ergative). When preceded by the 1st person absolutive marker, the 2nd person ergative suffix changes into -dé.
- ⁴) The 3rd person ergative marker is -ga when preceded by a 1st or 2nd person marker. After classifiers for a 3rd person argument, it surfaces as -a instead.
All possible combinations of absolutive and ergative suffixes are listed below for reference:
ergative → | none | 1.erg | 2.erg | 3.erg | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
absolutive ↓ | ||||||
1.abs | -na | -nàne | -ndé | -nga | ||
2.abs | -tá | -táne | -tátsé | -tága | ||
3.abs | I | -go | -gòne | -goké | -goa | |
II | -ta | -tàne | -tatsé | -tea | ||
III | -eatá | -eatáne | -eatátsé | -etêa | ||
IV | -ri | -rìne | -ritsé | -rea | ||
V | -tsú | -tsúne | -tsúké | -tsôa | ||
VI | -i | -ìne | -itsé¹ | -yea¹ | ||
VII | -tsa | -tsàne | -tsatsé | -tsea |
- ¹) These combinations coalesce with preceding monophthongal i (of any tone, most notably in the sensory evidential and in the interrogative mood) into -ìtsé and -ìya respectively.
Derivation
Adjectival prefixes
Like many other Western languages, Doayâu expresses adjectival meanings by means of derivational prefixes that can be added to any nominal. In theory, the morphemes of this class cannot appear independently, but the distinction between adjectival prefixes on the one hand and nominal roots which are commonly used as the first part of noun-noun compounds on the other hand seems to be weakening. For the most part, this entails the addition of new adjectival prefixes from formerly independent nouns, but as more and more roots can be used in both contexts, it is to be expected that the word class barrier will eventually become permeable in both directions. As it stands, there are too many adjectival prefixes for a full list to be given here; they will be included in the lexicon instead.
Adjectival prefixes precede the nominal root, but follow any possessive prefixes. A noun may take more than one adjectival prefix at a time; the prefixes are then normally understood as multiple properties simultaneously applying to the noun. However, a second, hierarchical interpretation is often possible as well, which treats the last prefix and the root as a unit, modified by the preceding prefix, which may itself be modified by another adjectival morpheme. Which of these interpretations is meant in a specific case is not indicated morphologically and must be deduced from context.
Adjectival prefixes are not affected by any morphophonological processes except for tone lowering before nasal consonants. However, some prefixes trigger nasal mutation of the first consonant in the root they attach to, dropping their own final vowel in the process; and those prefixes that do not trigger nasal mutation usually cause lenition of voiced plosives instead.
- yerisìlugo
- yeri-sìlugu-Ø
- wise-priest-ABS.SG
- itsindípe
- a-tsingì-típi-Ø
- 3SG-slender-daughter-ABS.SG
- bapòmàfutséyòfù
- bapo-màfu-tséyòfù-Ø
- strong-large-fortress-ABS.SG
Of note is the prefix gif(i)-, which can be added before adjectival prefixes (and also before verb stems) to reverse the meaning of the following morpheme. It cannot be attached to nouns directly. The final vowel of this prefix appears only before consonants; stem-initial y and instances of g deriving from earlier *w vocalise to i u before a monophthongal vowel, with which they combine into a diphthong.
- gifearisìlugo
- gifi-yeri-sìlugu-Ø
- NEG-wise-priest-ABS.SG
The meaning of adjectival prefixes can be intensified with the morpheme -sV- (glossed as 'very') directly following the prefix. V represents a copy of the original underlying stem vowel, or a copy of the second half of an underlying diphthong. After prefixes with the stem vowels o u, the morpheme appears as -fV-.
- gìrasagáku
- gìra-sV-gáku-Ø
- big-very-house-ABS.SG
- ùbùfuyùmuyá
- ùbù-sV-yùmuyá-Ø
- intelligent-very-person-ABS.SG
Another derivational addition to adjectival prefixes is the morpheme -isi- (glossed as exc for "excessive"), which indicates that the head noun has too much of the quality indicated by the prefix. It comes between the last adjectival prefix and the noun stem, and is usually interpreted to refer to all prefixes simultaneously. The morpheme-initial i deletes preceding i e, and combines with preceding o into ai; the morpheme-final i is deleted before i e y.
- gìraisigáku
- gìra-isi-gáku-Ø
- big-EXC-house-ABS.SG
Compounding and incorporation
A highly productive word-forming strategy in Doayâu is object incorporation, which is very useful for fine-tuning the semantics of a verb. This construction prefixes the absolutive argument of a transitive verb to the verb stem, decreasing the valency of the resulting verb by one and promoting the former ergative participant to absolutive. Pragmatically this results in backgrounding of the patient, and in shifting the semantic focus of the verb from the "what" to the "how" of the action. Several combinations of verb and incorporated noun have acquired an idiosyncratic meaning, making the process derivational rather than inflectional.
The incorporation of animate nouns triggers aspirate mutation of verb-initial consonants. This was historically caused by the characteristic absolutive singular suffix *-h that appeared on non-incorporated absolutives; when the default position for overt absolutives switched to post-verbal, pre-verbal patients were reanalysed as incorporated, and their suffix was analogically extended to all animate nouns appearing immediately before the verb root.
Inanimate nouns trigger lenition on verb-initial voiced plosives when incorporated.
- natâi "chest" + getsi- "to wash" > natáyetsi- "to bathe oneself"
- goe "saltwater fish" + púpu- "to hunt" > goafúpu- "to fish (in the sea)"
Derivational morphology
Doayâu features a wealth of derivational morphology, which can be piled up in an agglutinative fashion to add very specific combinations of shades of meaning. All of the morphemes in the following table attach as suffixes to an existing stem and extend it to a new one. The resulting stem, which may or may not be of a different type of speech, can then inflect like a regular noun or verb.
Verbalisers
VERB to VERB
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-reaya-¹ | e/a | low | v.tr | v (-1) | detr, pass | detransitiviser or passive; reduces valency by deleting the agent | < *-zèʔaje- |
-rə-² | ə | v | v (+1) | caus | causative; increases valency by adding an agent | < *-dza- | |
-mu- | u, low | v | v | tel | forms telic verbs | < *mu "completely" | |
-neo- | ea | i, low | v | v | dur | forms durative or habitual verbs | < *ňaje 'frequently' |
-tsilə̀-² | ə | v | v | iter | forms iterative verbs | < *tłàʔlà 'repeatedly' | |
-sèsì- | i | v | v | mir | indicates unexpected information | < *sèhtì 'surprisingly' | |
-ùsi-² | v | v.tr | emph | forms transitive verbs referring to physical actions with intensive semantics | ← Kp.L. -usi- | ||
-yesə-³ | ə | high | v | v ((+1)) | 'towards' | indicates movement towards the focus; increases valency of intransitives by adding a goal | < *-ʔihtsa- |
-agu- | v | v ((+1)) | 'away' | indicates movement away from the focus; increases valency of intransitives by adding a source | < *-ʔogu- |
- ¹) The i-grade form of this suffix is -reagi-.
- ²) Preceding u o are deleted; preceding e is changed to a.
- ³) The underlying |ə| surfaces as -a- before suffixes that trigger i- or u-grade, as -u- before labial consonants and before the rep evidential, and as -i- otherwise.
NOUN to VERB
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-ayV-, -´yV-¹ | (V) | n, pre | v.it | stat | forms stative verbs | < *-ʔajV- | |
-fu-, -fa-² | ə | strong | n | v.tr | dyn.1 | forms telic dynamic verbs, usually of a process associated with the base | < *-hpa- |
-má- | a | low | n | v.tr | dyn.2 | forms telic dynamic verbs, of which the base is the instrument by which the action is carried out | < *meʔ 'by means of' |
-tá- | a | n | v.it | dyn.3 | forms atelic dynamic verbs, which name a process associated with the base | < *tha- 'do, make' | |
-tûi- | ui | n, pre | v.it | 'become' | forms verbs of acquiring a property associated with the base | < *thuja- 'become' | |
-túp(ə)-³ | ə | n, pre | v.it+ | fact.1 | forms emphatic factitive verbs, of which the base is the result | ← Kp.L. tuku 'make, create' | |
-rə-⁴ | ə | n.i | v.tr | fact.2 | forms factitive verbs, of which the base is the result | < *-dza- | |
-ya-⁵ | i | n | v.it | lat | forms directional verbs, of which the base is the focus point | < *je- 'go, move' | |
-iba-⁶ | n | v.tr | loq | forms verbs of communication or thinking, of which the base is the absolutive argument | ← Kp.L. -iba- |
- ¹) Preceding i u are changed to e o. Preceding a is deleted, yielding low tone: -àyV-. After diphthongs, the suffix is shortened to -´yV- which triggers a raising tone contour.
- ²) -fa- appears before suffixes that trigger i- or u-grade; -fu- appears otherwise.
- ³) The morpheme-final |ə| surfaces as zero before vowels. When followed by a consonant, it surfaces as -a- before suffixes that trigger i- or u-grade, and as -u- otherwise.
- ⁴) The underlying |ə| surfaces as -a- before suffixes that trigger i- or u-grade, as -u- before labial consonants and before the rep evidential, and as -i- otherwise.
- ⁵) This suffix is normally -ya-, but it coalesces with following e a into -yea- and with following o into -yeo-.
- ⁶) Preceding i e are deleted; preceding o is changed to a.
Nominalisers
VERB to NOUN
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-i-¹ | i | v | n.a | ptcp | forms participle subject/agent nouns of class I or II (animate) | ← Kp.L. -i- | |
-pi- | i | v | n.a | neg.ptcp | forms negated participle subject/agent nouns of class I or II (animate) | ← Kp.L. -pi- | |
-aba-² | v.tr | n.a.I | agt.1 | forms habitual agent nouns of class I (animate) | ← Kp.L. -ab-/-ãm- | ||
-té- | e | v.tr | n.a.I | agt.2 | forms emphatic or honorific agent nouns of class I (animate) | ← Ìl. -té- | |
-rù-, -ù-³ | u, low | v.tr | n.i.VII | instr | forms instrument nouns of class VII (inanimate) | < *-dzù-/-gù- | |
-lì- | i | i, low | v | n.i.IV | vn | forms abstract action nouns of class IV (inanimate) | possibly ← Kp.L. -li- 'acc' |
-sa-, -ha-⁴ | ə | v.tr | n | res.1 | forms resultative nouns of various classes | < *-ła-/-xa- | |
-mù- | low | v | n.i | res.2 | forms factitive result nouns of class VII (inanimate) | possibly ← Kp.L. -ũ- | |
-pa- | v | n.i.III | fact | forms factitive result nouns of class III (inanimate) | (unknown) | ||
-yari- | i | i | v.tr | n.a.I | pat | forms human undergoer nouns of class I (animate) | (unknown) |
-le- | e/a | high | v | n.i.III | loc.1 | forms specific location nouns of class III (inanimate) | < *-ʔle- |
-rè- | e/a | low | v | n.i.IV | loc.2 | forms generic location and area nouns of class IV (inanimate) | < *-zè- |
- ¹) Preceding i e are deleted; preceding o is changed to a.
- ²) Preceding i u are changed to e o; preceding a and |ə| are deleted.
- ³) -ù- appears when added to a- and o-stems, forming a diphthong -au-. -rù- appears elsewhere.
- ⁴) -ha- occurs after o- and u-stems, -sa- occurs elsewhere.
When added to a verb stem that triggers prenasalisation, the agt.2 affix becomes -ndé-, the fact affix becomes -mba-, and the loc.1 affix becomes -nde-.
NOUN to NOUN
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-lugV- | (V) | n, pre | n.a.I | 'person' | forms human nouns of class I which refer to a person associated with the base (animate) | < *-lawV- | |
-àma-¹ | n, pre | n.a.I | 'person' | forms human nouns of class I which refer to a person associated with the base (animate) | ← Kp.L. -ab-/-ãm- | ||
-la- | ə | n, pre | n.a | 'man' | forms animate nouns of class I or II which refer to a male person or animal associated with the base (animate) | < *ľa "man" | |
-rì- | i | i, low | n, pre | n.a | 'woman' | forms animate nouns of class I or II which refer to a female person or animal associated with the base (animate) | < *-zì- |
-yaV-, -yV́-² | i | n, pre | n.a | 'child' | forms animate nouns of class I or II which refer to a descendant of the base (animate) | < *-jaʔV- | |
-saba- | n | n.a | 'owner' | forms animate nouns of class I or II which refer to a possessor of the base (animate) | ← Kp.L. -sab- | ||
-yo- | u | u | n, pre | n.a | aug | forms honorific and augmentative nouns of class I or II (animate) | < *-du- |
-fí- | i | n | n | dim | forms diminutive nouns without changing noun class | < *phiji 'seed' | |
-kì- | n | n | simil | forms nouns of various classes which are in some relevant way similar to the base | ← Kp.L. -kĩ- | ||
-òfù- | del | n | n.i.III | 'made_of' | forms class III nouns which are made of the referent of the base (inanimate) | < *-òhxù- | |
-mau | low | n, pre | n.i | 'place' | forms toponyms of class III or IV (inanimate) | ← Kp.L. mau 'place' | |
-le- | e/a | high | n, pre | n.i.III | loc.1 | forms specific location nouns of class III (inanimate) | < *-ʔle- |
-rè- | e/a | low | n, pre | n.i.IV | loc.2 | forms generic location and area nouns of class IV (inanimate) | < *-zè- |
-ká- | a | n, pre | n.i | 'thing' | forms inanimate nouns of various meanings, belonging into class IV, V or VII | < *-kha- | |
-tù-, -pù-³ | u, low | n, pre | n.i.IV | abstr, coll.1 | forms abstract and collective nouns of class IV (inanimate) | < *-tu-/-ku-, *-tsù-/-kù- | |
-nà- | low | n | n.a | coll.2 | forms collective nouns of class I or II (animate) | ← Kp.L. -nã- 'many' |
- ¹) Preceding i u are changed to e o; preceding a and |ə| are deleted.
- ²) -yaV- occurs after i- and u-stems. -yV́- occurs elsewhere, with stem-final diphthongs being copied to the suffix as e or o.
- ³) -tù- appears after i- and e-stems, -pù- appears elsewhere.
ADJECTIVAL to NOUN
Most of the above noun-to-noun nominalisers can also be used directly with adjectival prefixes. Another productive way to form stand-alone nominals from those prefixes, which carry semantic content but cannot normally appear on their own, has been innovated from compounding them with a (historical) cataphoric pronoun and the classifier of the desired noun class. Since this origin has become obscured a fair bit, all the resulting affixes are given below for convenience.
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-bo- | o/a | i | pre | n.a.I | nom.I | forms class I nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (animate) | < *guwo |
-ìta-¹ | ə | pre | n.a.II | nom.II | forms class II nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (animate) | < *gita | |
-geatá- | a | i | pre | n.i.III | nom.III | forms class III nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (inanimate) | < *gijatha |
-ìri-¹ | i | pre | n.i.IV | nom.IV | forms class IV nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (inanimate) | < *gizi | |
-ùtsú-² | pre | n.i.V | nom.V | forms class V nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (inanimate) | < *gutshu | ||
-ye- | i | i | pre | n.i.VI | nom.VI | forms class VI nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (inanimate) | < *gije |
-ìtsa-¹ | ə | pre | n.i.VII | nom.VII | forms class VII nouns with the characteristic indicated by the base (inanimate) | < *gitsa |
- ¹) Preceding i e are deleted; preceding o is changed to a.
- ²) Preceding u o are deleted; preceding e is changed to a.
LOCATIVE SUFFIXES
From the Lukpanic substratum, Doayâu has adopted the habit of referencing people or things by adding a locative suffix to a place, person or object that is associated with them. The table below lists the most common derivational suffixes of this type, most of them descended from PCW postpositions. In traditional usage, locative suffixes attach to the oblique singular of the base; however, in recent times it has become possible to use the root instead.
suffix | ablaut | triggers | from | to | gloss | meaning | etymology |
-sí- | ə | n.obl | n | 'inside' | forms nouns of various classes associated with a location inside or among the base | < *sàʔ 'inside' | |
-mea- | ea | n.obl | n | 'near' | forms nouns of various classes associated with a location near the base | < *mèľà 'along' | |
-ní- | ə | n.obl | n | 'above' | forms nouns of various classes associated with a location above the base | < *naʔ 'above' | |
-lí- | ə | n.obl | n | 'under' | forms nouns of various classes associated with a location under the base | < *ľaʔ 'under' | |
-pu- | n.obl | n | 'behind' | forms nouns of various classes associated with a location behind the base | < *guku 'behind' | ||
-nau- | n.obl | n.a | 'approaching' | forms animate nouns of class I or II associated with movement towards the base | < *noʔu 'up towards' | ||
-ádi-, -gádi-¹ | i | n.obl | n | 'facing_towards' | forms nouns of various classes that are oriented towards the base | < *gèʔdzi 'towards' | |
-yau- | n.obl | n | 'facing_away' | forms nouns of various classes that are oriented away from the base | < *jeʔogu 'away from' | ||
-la- | ə | high | n.obl | n | ben | forms nouns of various classes, which are beneficial or instrumental in relation to the base | < *dła 'for' |
- ¹) -gádi- appears after diphthongs. -ádi- appears otherwise; it coalesces with preceding u into -ôadi-.
Syntax
Since verbal inflection keeps track of up to two core arguments of a clause in terms of role, person, and noun class, and since nominals themselves exhibit case marking, constituent order in Doayâu is relatively free. Under the influence of the Lukpanic language family, which is largely head-initial, the default sentence pattern has shifted to (T)VSO, though many other orderings can also be selected on the basis of pragmatic considerations.
The noun phrase
The noun phrase minimally consists of a head noun or pronoun, often accompanied by one or several modifiers. Possessors, demonstratives, and quantifiers (in this order) generally precede the head, while nouns in apposition, postpositional phrases, prepositional phrases, and relative clauses (in this order) follow it.
Possessive phrases
Possessors are normally nouns marked for the oblique case, which can take modifiers of their own. In case a possessor is to be specified with a "heavy" postposed modifier (more than one appositional noun, an adpositional phrase, or a relative clause), the possessive phrase itself is usually expressed within a relative clause modifying the possessed noun, with the possessive relationship indicated at the main NP level via possessive prefixes alone.
- nùmìnau
- ni-mìna-ù
- 1SG-mother-OBL.SG
- ayealí
- a-gala-í
- 3SG-child-ABS.PL
- silágau
- si-dága-ù
- 1PL-tribe-OBL.SG
- asâipufu
- a-sâipi-fu
- 3SG-god-OBL.PL
- sìyá
- sa-ìye-á
- 3PL.ANIM-name-ABS.PL
- dagafu
- da-go-fu
- this-I-OBL.PL
- rugo
- ri-go
- two-I
- dufoayufu
- dufoayu-fu
- fisherman-OBL.PL
- tséyeatá
- tséye-eatá-Ø
- that.EMPH-III-ABS.SG
- sànêa
- sa-nêa-Ø
- 3PL.ANIM-boat-ABS.SG
Appositional nouns
Doayâu does not have adjectives as a distinct word class. Many basic adjectival meanings are expressed by derivational prefixes, others are conveyed by stative verbs. As a third option, most nouns can be used adjectivally by being placed in apposition to another noun, immediately following their head. This strategy is especially common with names and titles. Certain nouns occur almost exclusively in this position, most of them loans or transparent derivations, but even these can be used as stand-alone nominals.
Until recently, appositional nouns used to be cast in the oblique case. While this remains grammatical in many circumstances, it is becoming old-fashioned, and speakers now tend to favour agreement with the head in case and number.
- Fólu
- Fólu-Ø
- Poalu-ABS.SG
- sâipuyu
- sâipi-yu-Ø
- god-HON-ABS.SG
A second construction that is syntactically parallel to noun apposition uses participial modifiers, which can be derived from verbs with the suffix -i (negated: -pi). Such participles are nominal in nature and prototypically refer to the subject or agent of the base verb; when the passiviser -reaya- is present as well, they can also refer to the patient of transitive verbs. Unlike regular nouns in apposition, participles do not exhibit agreement.
- máluyo
- máluyu-Ø
- warrior-ABS.SG
- suguyai
- suguya-i
- fight-PTCP
- irí
- iri-´
- arrow-ABS.PL
- fáfátìreayapi
- fáfáti-reaya-pi
- shoot-PASS-NEG.PTCP
Adpositional phrases
There are two distinct types of adpositions in Doayâu, which are lexically specified for the headedness of their phrases. The older type is a class of postpositional enclitics, which govern the oblique case for the noun they attach to. The more recently innovated type uses prepositions that are morphologically independent and govern the absolutive case for their object; many of these prepositions are derived from nouns in the oblique case, sometimes with an enclitic postposition.
- késabaumá
- késaba-ù=amá
- knife-OBL.SG=INSTR
- bàyu
- bàyu
- on
- neomo
- neomo-Ø
- dune-ABS.SG
Normally all adpositional phrases follow the head of the NP. If both postpositional and prepositional constructions are found within the same NP, the postpositional phrase comes first. In the most formal and archaic registers (mostly in a religious context), postpositional phrases may also precede the head noun, coming after any possessives, demonstratives or quantifiers.
The fact that postpositions are clitics becomes visible when a prepositional phrase is nested within a postpositional phrase:
- Dìlìnearugo
- dìlì-neo-ri-go
- fly-DUR-SENS-I.ABS
- màfuyurau
- [màfu-dure-ù
- [wide-sky-OBL.SG
- dùtsù
- [dùtsù
- [over
- murayutuge
- murayu-Ø]=tuge]
- ocean-ABS.SG]=LOC]
- aufayaba
- aufayaba-Ø
- albatross-ABS.SG
Basic clause structure
The minimal clause in Doayâu consists of an inflected verb, which can form a valid sentence on its own because subject and transitive object are always referenced on the verb and can thus be omitted. An exception is formed by some bi- and trivalent verbs that lexically require an oblique complement noun phrase, which cannot be dropped. (Such verbs are indicated in the lexicon by a + sign after the transitivity value; for instance, v.mt+ denotes a monotransitive verb with a required complement.)
Complement NPs usually precede the verb; ergative and absolutive arguments usually follow it. If several core arguments are present on the same side of the verb, the order is invariably ergative - oblique - absolutive. Adjuncts (i.e. optional NPs in adverbial function) can appear on either side of the core argument group; most commonly they go furthest from the verb.
The first overt noun phrase to the left of the verb, if there is any, is generally assumed to be topicalised, and thus interpreted as old information. Conversely, the last overt constituent of a clause is understood to be in focus position, receiving contrastive emphasis.
- Nêa
- nêa-Ø
- ship-ABS.SG
- biusirùpùyetêa.
- biusi-ri-pù-eatá-a
- build-SENS-AFF-III.ABS-3.ERG
- Biusiretêa
- biusi-ri-eatá-a
- build-SENS-III.ABS-3.ERG
- (dasi)
- da-si
- PROX-ERG.PL
- nêa.
- nêa-Ø
- ship-ABS.SG
- Tâurita
- tâu-ri-ta
- die-SENS-II.ABS
- tsófú
- tsófú-Ø
- deer-ABS.SG
- bàyu
- bàyu
- on
- pùtau.
- pùtau-Ø
- path-ABS.SG
- Òmokabai
- òmokaba-ì
- assassin-ERG.SG
- tumeagaure
- tumeagà-ù=re
- noble-OBL.SG=towards
- késaba
- késaba-Ø
- dagger-ABS.SG
- fáfátugagoa
- fáfáti-ga-go-a
- throw-ASS-I.ABS-3.ERG
Subordination
Complement and adverbial clauses
Complement clauses, i.e. clauses that work as an argument of a verb, are syntactically normal clauses which are followed by the subordinating pronoun yari, which governs class IV agreement on the verb. Complement clauses may only appear in an absolutive role.
Adverbial clauses are formed with the same conjunction, cast in the oblique case. In the basic form yarù, this can indicate a wide variety of meanings that generally have a sort of circumstantial flavour. More precise meanings can be specified with a range of suffixes that derive from PCW postpositions; the most widespread of these suffixed forms are given in the table below. Note that in casual speech the initial ya- is often dropped, especially when a suffix is used.
complementiser | |
yari | absolutive argument of verb |
relational adverbials | |
yarù | circumstantial ("as") |
yarûi | conditional ("if") |
yarúgai | causal ("because") |
yarùla | purposive ("for, so that, in order to") |
yarôamá | instrumental ("with, by") |
temporal adverbials | |
yarùtuge | punctual ("when") |
yarùsí | simultaneous ("while, during") |
yarùnau | anterior ("before") |
yarùpu | posterior ("after") |
With complement clauses and relational adverbials, the subordinator always comes last, but temporal clauses have been remodeled as head-initial on analogy with the now dominant prepositional nominal adjuncts.
The internal word order in both complement and adverbial clauses may be either verb-first or verb-last, but not verb-medial. Likewise, both types of subclauses may be placed in their matrix clause either initially (as an extraposited topic) or finally; in the latter case all other core arguments of the verb may only appear in pre-verbal position.
Examples:
- Murayùtuge
- murayu-ù=tuge
- ocean-OBL.SG=in
- gítîu
- gítiu-´
- whale-ABS.PL
- dausuluta
- dausi-lu-ta
- swim-EVID-II.ABS
- yari
- yari-Ø
- SUB-ABS
- daidàmurìne.
- daidə-mu-Ø-ri-ne
- see-TEL-DIR-IV.ABS-1.ERG
- Bufotsaiditàne
- bufu-daidə-Ø-ta-ne
- wolf-see-DIR-II.ABS-1.ERG
- yarù
- yari-ù
- SUB-OBL
- radeogùna.
- radeogu-Ø-na
- run_away-DIR-1.ABS
- Táyaritsé
- tá-ga-ri-tsé
- do-ASS-IV.ABS-2.ERG
- yarûi
- yari-ù=i
- SUB-OBL=if
- yeogugàpùtága
- yeogu-ga-pù-tá-a
- leave-ASS-AFF-2.ABS-3.ERG
- dàrì.
- da-àrè-ì
- 2SG-wife-ERG
- Yarùpu
- yari-ù=ùpu
- SUB-OBL=at
- íyesàmugago
- íyesə-mu-ga-go
- come-TEL-ASS-I.ABS
- pèyogú,
- pèyogú-Ø
- lord-ABS.SG
- tupíbayatsea
- tupíba-ga-tsa-a
- perform-ASS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
- yautì
- yauté-ì
- high_priest-ERG.SG
- fà
- fà
- then
- mìnda.
- mìnda-Ø
- ceremony-ABS.SG
Relative clauses
Relative clauses usually appear directly after their head noun. They are formed like normal verb-final clauses that are introduced with the topic pronoun gi-, which replaces the relativised noun in the subclause, regularly inflecting for noun class and case. The matrix clause is usually arranged in such a way that the relativised noun is the only argument on its side of the verb. If the verbs of matrix clause and subclause share the same root, whichever comes second is generally replaced by the dummy verb tá- "to do".
- Galàrì
- galàrì-ì
- girl-ERG.SG
- tsugànauritsea
- tsuga-nau-ri-tsa-a
- go-upward-SENS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
- kápa
- kápa-Ø
- tree-ABS.SG
- gitsa
- gi-tsa-Ø
- REL-VII-ABS.SG
- tsagi
- tsagi
- also
- layè
- lai-ì
- boy-ERG.SG
- téritsea.
- tá-ri-tsa-a
- do-SENS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
Relativised constituents are often extraposited to the beginning of the sentence, and thus effectively moved outside of the main clause.
- Kápa
- kápa-Ø
- tree-ABS.SG
- gitsa
- gi-tsa-Ø
- REL-VII-ABS.SG
- layè
- lai-ì
- boy-ERG.SG
- tsugànauritsea,
- tsuga-nau-ri-tsa-a
- go-upward-SENS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
- galàrì
- galàrì-ì
- girl-ERG.SG
- tsagi
- tsagi
- also
- tsétsa
- tsé-tsa-Ø
- that-VII-ABS.SG
- téritsea.
- tá-ri-tsa-a
- do-SENS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
An alternative to relative clauses is a participial construction, which is, however, only available for topic-fronted arguments. The agent of the participle verb is demoted to the oblique case, with a clitic instrumental postposition. It should be noted that the "participle" suffix is derivational in nature and creates a word that would more often be used as a free-standing noun than as a modifier, and that using a dummy verb is not possible in this construction.
- Kápa
- kápa-Ø
- tree-ABS.SG
- tsugànaureayai
- tsuga-nau-reaya-i
- go-upward-PASS-PTCP
- layôamá
- lai-ù=amá
- boy-OBL.SG=INSTR
- tsugànauritsea
- tsuga-nau-ri-tsa-a
- go-upward-SENS-VII.ABS-3.ERG
- galàrì.
- galàrì-ì
- girl-ERG.SG
Samples
The horse and the sheep
- Main article: The horse and the sheep
Sàrì bàyu tau daidùgùtea mâugú. Túlugasùgùtea ìrì tugauta, lítùgùtea galai ruyùta, garadùgùtea lai naupùta. Sakáyauní baumùreayàgùta màtsauta mâu.
Sàrì maugùre dadìgàrea giri: "Uyàyàna yarùtuge dú yùmuyési mâugú tátéritea."
Gataibàgàrea furá maugì sàrùre giri: "Rêayesàmaundé, fà dìri tsufólùpùritsé: Uyàyàna yarùtuge púpàreagirita tsa yèsafàreagirita sàre, gita radatsu mítànìrita. Yùmuyési pùtolúfarea gearùmá tátólurea dagaupu. Gatunà ge daidalurea. Fà tsagi yùmuyófu gugutûisèsìluká!"
Yarùpu reagàrea giri, sàre radeogùgùta rò tále.
Interlinear gloss
- Sàrì
- sàre-ì
- horse-ERG.SG
- bàyu
- bàyu
- on
- tau
- tau-Ø
- hill-ABS.SG
- daidùgùtea
- daida-gù-ta-a
- see-REP-II.ABS-3.ERG
- mâugú.
- mâu-ú
- sheep-ABS.PL
- Túlugasùgùtea
- túlugasa-gù-ta-a
- wool_cut-REP-II.ABS-3.ERG
- ìrì
- ìrè-ì
- woman-ERG.SG
- tugauta,
- tugo-ù-ta-Ø
- one-ORD-II-ABS.SG
- lítùgùtea
- líta-gù-ta-a
- milk-REP-II.ABS-3.ERG
- galai
- gala-ì
- child-ERG.SG
- ruyùta,
- ri-ù-ta-Ø
- two-ORD-II-ABS.SG
- garadùgùtea
- garada-gù-ta-a
- kill-REP-II.ABS-3.ERG
- lai
- la-ì
- man-ERG.SG
- naupùta.
- naupu-ù-ta-Ø
- three-ORD-II-ABS.SG
- Sakáyauní
- sa-káya-ù=ní
- 3PL.ANIM-fire-OBL.SG=on
- baumùreayàgùta
- bau-mu-reaya-gù-ta
- cook-TEL-PASS-REP-II.ABS
- màtsauta
- màtsa-ù-ta-Ø
- four-ORD-II-ABS.SG
- mâu.
- mâu-Ø
- sheep-ABS.SG
- Sàrì
- sàre-ì
- horse-ERG.SG
- maugùre
- mâu-ù=re
- sheep-OBL.SG=to
- dadìgàrea
- dadi-gà-ri-a
- say-REP-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- giri:
- gi-ri-Ø
- TOP-IV-ABS
- "Uyàyàna
- uyàya-Ø-na
- be_in_pain-DIR-1SG
- yarùtuge
- yari-ù=tuge
- SUB-OBL=at
- dú
- dú
- like_this
- yùmuyési
- yùmuyá-si
- human-ERG.PL
- mâugú
- mâu-ú
- sheep-ABS.PL
- tátéritea."
- tátá-ri-ta-a
- use-SENS-II.ABS-3.ERG
- Gataibàgàrea
- gataiba-gà-ri-a
- answer-REP-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- furá
- furá
- however
- maugì
- mâu-ì
- sheep-ERG.SG
- sàrùre
- sàre-ù=re
- horse-OBL.SG=to
- giri:
- gi-ri-Ø
- TOP-IV-ABS
- "Rêayesàmaundé,
- rêayesa-mau-na-tsé
- listen-OPT-1.ABS-2.ERG
- fà
- fà
- and_then
- dìri
- dì-ri-Ø
- this.EMPH-IV-ABS
- tsufólùpùritsé:"
- tsufá-lu-pù-ri-tsé
- find_out-EVID-AFF-IV.ABS-2.ERG
- "Uyàyàna
- uyàya-Ø-na
- be_in_pain-DIR-1SG
- yarùtuge
- yari-ù=tuge
- SUB-OBL=at
- púpàreagirita
- púpa-reaya-ri-ta
- hunt-PASS-SENS-II.ABS
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- yèsafàreagirita
- yèsafa-reaya-ri-ta
- eat_meat-PASS-SENS-II.ABS
- sàre,
- sàre-Ø
- horse-ABS.SG
- gita
- gi-ta-Ø
- REL-II-ABS
- radatsu
- rada-tsu
- quick-ADV
- mítànìrita."
- mítànì-ri-ta
- run-SENS-II.ABS
- "Yùmuyési
- yùmuyá-si
- human-ERG.PL
- pùtolúfarea
- pùta-lu-fa-ri-a
- find_one's_way-EVID-NEG-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- gearùmá
- geari-ù=má
- what-OBL=by
- tátólurea
- tátá-lu-ri-a
- use-EVID-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- dagaupu."
- da-gaupu-Ø
- 2SG-speed-ABS.SG
- "Gatunà
- gatunà
- next_year
- ge
- ge
- but
- daidalurea."
- daida-lu-ri-a
- see-EVID-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- "Fà
- fà
- and_then
- tsagi
- tsagi
- also
- yùmuyófu
- yùmuyá-fu
- human-OBL.PL
- gugutûisèsìluká!"
- guga-tûi-sèsì-lu-tá
- slave-become-MIR-EVID-2.ABS
- "Yarùpu
- yari-ù=ùpu
- SUB-OBL=after
- reagàrea
- rea-gà-ri-a
- hear-REP-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- giri,
- gi-ri-Ø
- TOP-IV-ABS
- sàre
- sàre-Ø
- horse-ABS.SG
- radeogùgùta
- radeogu-gù-ta
- run_away-REP-II.ABS
- rò
- rò
- into
- tále.
- tále-Ø
- grassland-ABS.SG
When truth is lost
Fílitùluri mùnduyù mítàmau figautù yarù, itsérìne súsàmùmaurìne yari. Famauní furá burìna, fálai tsa naure dadirea giri:
"Loagù yalùma súsàmùmauritsé. Yarùpu rátàraluri ai figautù, rátàraluri apúpaugûifa, gìmaure fà yeogugíri yari daidalúfùpùrìne."
Interlinear gloss
- Fílitùluri
- fíli-tûi-lu-ri
- hidden-become-EVID-IV.ABS
- mùnduyù
- mùnduyù
- all_around
- mítàmau
- mítàmau-Ø
- world-ABS.SG
- figautù
- figautù-Ø
- truth-ABS.SG
- yarù,
- yari-ù
- SUB-OBL
- itsérìne
- itsá-Ø-ri-ne
- strive-DIR-IV.ABS-1.ERG
- súsàmùmaurìne
- súsà-mu-Ø-mau-ri-ne
- search-TEL-DIR-OPT-IV.ABS-1.ERG
- yari.
- yari-Ø
- SUB-ABS
- Famauní
- fá=mə-ù=ní
- every=place-OBL.SG=on
- furá
- furá
- however
- burìna,
- buri-Ø-na
- ask-DIR-1.ABS
- fálai
- fá=la-ì
- every=man-ERG.SG
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- naure
- nau=re
- 1SG.OBL=to
- dadirea
- dadi-Ø-ri-a
- say-DIR-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- giri:
- gi-ri-Ø
- TOP-IV-ABS
- "Loagù
- loagù
- towards
- yalùma
- yalu=mə-Ø
- other=place-ABS.SG
- súsàmùmauritsé.
- súsà-mu-Ø-mau-ri-tsé
- search-TEL-DIR-OPT-IV.ABS-2.ERG
- Yarùpu
- yari-ù=ùpu
- SUB-OBL=at
- rátàraluri
- rátàri-lu-ri
- fade-EVID-IV.ABS
- ai
- ai
- because
- figautù,
- figautù-Ø
- truth-ABS.SG
- rátàraluri
- rátàri-lu-ri
- fade-EVID-IV.ABS
- apúpaugûifa,
- a-púpau-ù=ifa
- 3SG-trace-OBL.SG=without
- gìmaure
- gi=mə-ù=re
- which=place-OBL.SG=to
- fà
- fà
- and_then
- yeogugíri
- yeogu-Ø-gí-ri
- leave-ASS-Q-IV.ABS
- yari
- yari-Ø
- SUB-ABS
- daidalúfùpùrìne."
- daida-lu-fu-pù-ri-ne
- see-EVID-NEG-AFF-IV.ABS-1.ERG
The wooden dog
Gigatûisèsìrugo tsàbò likésai bàyu turù. Dìnèfarita tsa ngâupurita lípòfùmùrà, gita tsí rìyau galau atsúpú yàgirita. Fígoagurìya fà gìralugòfùfala, gì roatsilàri tsa bagasìreagirì tsa fárìreagirì, nifátûi yâi gì nàgù nìmi tsóturì. Dàmauní, bàyu màfùnâibi bágalàyai, rìgàyarugo mìnayo yeràyai gugau akénu kápágì ga yàgèni kâu tsoa dùmàyariri. Sìgìbalúfarea furá fárù yaupú tsa táfâi yarù, tuilurùpùgo tsíri tsógauga nire, dagai akísífuyeogu ngiriutsú dearagurutsôa.
Interlinear gloss
- Gigatûisèsìrugo
- giga-tûi-sèsì-ri-go
- be_alive-become-MIR-SENS-NC1.ABS
- tsàbò
- tsàbò
- again
- likésai
- likésai-Ø
- statue-ABS.SG
- bàyu
- bàyu
- at
- turù.
- turù-Ø
- corner-ABS.SG
- Dìnèfarita
- dìnèfə-ri-ta
- wake_up-SENS-NC2.ABS
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- ngâupurita
- ngâupu-ri-ta
- bark-SENS-NC2.ABS
- lìpòfùmùrà,
- lìpa-òfù-mùrà-Ø
- wood-made_of-dog-ABS.SG,
- gita
- gi-ta
- REL-NC2.ABS
- tsí
- tsí
- earlier
- rìyau
- rìyau
- between
- galau
- galə-ù
- child-OBL.SG
- atsúpú
- a-tsúpo-´
- 3SG.POSS-arm-ABS.PL
- yàgirita.
- yàgi-ri-ta
- rest-SENS-NC2.ABS
- Fígoagurìya
- fíga-agu-ri-i-a
- breathe-away-SENS-NC6.ABS-3.ERG
- fà
- fà
- and_then
- gìralugòfùfala,
- gìra-lugo-òfù-fala-Ø
- big-smoke-made_of-cloud-ABS.SG
- gì
- gi-i
- REL-NC6.ABS
- roatsilàrì
- roa-tsilə`-ri-i
- drift-ITER-SENS-NC6.ABS
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- bagasìreagirì
- bagasə-reaya-ri-i
- split-DETR-SENS-NC6.ABS
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- fárìreagirì,
- fárə-reaya-ri-i
- gather-DETR-SENS-NC6.ABS,
- nifátûi
- nifá-tûi-i
- fog-become-PTCP
- yâi
- yâi
- finally
- gì
- gi-i
- REL-NC6.ABS
- nàgù
- nàgù
- next_to
- nìmi
- nìmi-Ø
- wall-ABS.SG
- tsóturì.
- tsótu-ri-i
- settle-SENS-NC6.ABS
- Dàmauní,
- damə-ù=ní
- that_place-OBL.SG=on,
- bàyu
- bàyu
- at
- màfùnâibi
- màfu-nâibi-Ø
- broad-platform-ABS.SG
- bágalàyai,
- bága-làye-i
- snow-be_under-PTCP,
- rìgàyarugo
- rìgàye-ri-go
- stand-SENS-NC1.ABS
- mìnayo
- mìnayo-Ø
- lady-ABS.SG
- yeràyai
- yerə-ayV-i
- venerable-STAT-PTCP
- gugau
- gi-gau
- REL-NC1.OBL
- akénu
- a-kénu-Ø
- 3SG.POSS-face-ABS.SG
- kápágì
- kápágè-i
- mock-PTCP
- ga
- ga
- but
- yàgèni
- yàgène-i
- seduce-PTCP
- kâu
- kâu
- like
- tsoa
- tsoa-Ø
- peach-ABS.SG
- dùmàyariri.
- dùmə-ayV-ri-ri
- red-STAT-SENS-NC4.ABS
- Sìgìbalúfarea
- sìgìba-lu-fə-ri-a
- consider-EVID-NEG-NC4.ABS-3.ERG
- furá
- furá
- however
- fárù
- fá-rù
- all-NC4.OBL
- yaupú
- yaupu-´
- ritual-ABS.PL
- tsa
- tsa
- and
- táfâi
- táfai-´
- distance-ABS.PL
- yarù,
- yari-ù
- SUB-OBL,
- tuilurùpùgo
- tuilu-ri-pù-go
- smile-SENS-AFF-NC1.ABS
- tsíri
- tsíri
- instead
- tsógauga
- tsé-gau=a
- MED-NC1.OBL=for
- nire,
- nire
- unexpectedly
- dagai
- da-gai
- PROX-NC1.ERG
- akísífuyeogu
- a-kísí-fu=yeogu
- 3SG.POSS-cheek-OBL.PL=from
- ngiriutsú
- ngiriutsú
- fine_cloth-ABS.SG
- dearagurutsôa.
- deari-agu-ri-tsú-a
- put-away-SENS-NC5.ABS-3.ERG
(This text was written on Dec 29, 2010 as part of Conlang Relay 18. Translated from Sean Anderson's West Germanic {broken link → partial backup}.)
Proverbs
- Difé
- difé-Ø
- water-ABS.SG
- gì
- gi-i-Ø
- REL-VI-ABS.SG
- débutérì
- débu-tá-ri-i
- ebb-DYN.3-SENS-VI
- fanêagá
- fá=nêa-´
- all=boat-ABS.PL
- yàgupuretêa.
- yàgupu-ri-eatá-a
- suck-SENS-III.ABS-3.ERG
(Changes in circumstances affect everybody.)
- Gùmùtuge
- gùmu-ù=tuge
- city-OBL.SG=LOC
- lósàreayàgêatá
- lósa-reaya-gù-eatá
- trade-PASS-REP-III.ABS
- lìtseayàsôagá,
- lìtsi=àyà=sôa-´
- many=splendid=goods-ABS.PL
- dufoayui
- dufoayo-ì
- fisherman-ERG.SG
- furá
- however
- however
- yèsí
- yèsa-´
- food-ABS.PL
- sápalùpùrea.
- sápa-lu-pù-ri-a
- bring-EVID-AFF-IV.ABS-3.ERG
(Keep to that which you're sure you can rely on.)
- Kôató
- kôato-´
- celebration-ABS.PL
- sápalurea
- sápa-lu-ri-a
- bring-EVID-IV.ABS-3.ERG
- fanùyèmisi:
- fá=nùyèmi-si
- all=guest-ERG.PL
- íyesàmulugo
- íyesə-mu-lu-go
- come-TEL-EVID-I.ABS
- yarù
- yari-ù
- SUB-OBL
- tupugau
- tupi-go-ù
- some-I-OBL
- dogá,
- da-go-´
- PROX-I-ABS.PL
- yalugau
- yalu-go-ù
- other-I-OBL
- furá
- furá
- however
- yeogùmulugo
- yeogu-mu-lu-go
- leave-TEL-EVID-I.ABS
- yarù.
- yari-ù
- SUB-OBL